• Skip to primary navigation
  • Skip to main content
  • Skip to footer

Captain Psychology

  • Topics
  • Notes
  • Videos
  • Syllabus

Learning

April 5, 2023 by ktangen

What Is Context

Context is a version of pattern recognition. In writing, it is how words are woven together. It is the combination of all the word connections. In perception, context is finding yourself in relation to the world around you. It is how you, objects and places relate to each other. Context is the whole that is larger than the sum of its parts.

People are very good at tracking their environments. We collect lots of data on where we are, how close we are to objects and the spatial relationships between everything. We use this information to decide what to do in any given circumstance.

Context cues allow us to discriminate between a baseball field, a synagogue and our backyard patio. The cues tell us that this place feels like home and that place feels strange and uncomfortable.

As it turns out, we recall best when the encoding and decoding contexts are the same. If we learn in a quiet environment, we do best when we recall in a quiet environment. We entered the information with quiet cues, so it is easiest to retrieve them when those cues are present.

The general principle of matching learning and recall contexts is called the Encoding Specificity Principle. Endel Tulving, a Canadian psychologist, found that performance on cued recall of word lists was improved when the encoding and decoding contexts matched. If you learn a list in one location, you remember it best if you are in that location when you are given a cue to remember a particular word.

Tulving suggests that you need to be in “retrieval mode” during recall. You must be trying to use all the cues you have available to help you remember. If you know the material, you don’t need any cues. But if you’ve ever taken a class in a quiet room and find yourself looking around hoping an answer will pop into your head, you probably should not have studied with your music blaring.

In the 1970s, educators were freaking out. They were worried that students wouldn’t perform well if they didn’t take the test in the exact classroom they learned it in. What if you have to take the SATs in the cafeteria? What if learning it in school meant you couldn’t apply it in real life?

The Encoding Specificity Principle isn’t that exact. We do best when the contexts match, and less well when they are less similar, but that isn’t the whole story. It depends on the material you are learning and the way you test its recall.

In recognition tests, context and environment cues don’t help much. Since all the information you need is present on the test, you don’t need other cues as much. In free recall (remember them in any order), environmental cues are more helpful, particularly for episodic memories.

People who go back to the house they grew up in are often flooded with memories. Everything seems to rush back. It can be overwhelming when the home has the same lighting, furniture, smells and sounds. People experience this overload at class reunions, revisiting battlefields, and visiting a previous employer.

On a smaller scale, context clues help us learn. When reading a book, we might remember what is at the top of page 34. In a classroom, we might remember a point because the prof circled it with colored chalk. During a game, we find the home team advantage is our familiarity with the court.

Our senses provide us with both top-down and down-up processing. Down-up is inductive. We sense the world around us and build those sensations into complex patterns. We also use deductive reasoning to process things top-down. We look ahead to see what is coming our way.

It seems like every hunting season comes with a story of a tragedy or a near miss for shooting a fellow hunter. Typically, one hunter is stationary and the other drives the deer toward him for an easy kill. Unfortunately, the stationary hunter might shoot at movements in the bushes only to discover that he has wounded or killed his partner. If we expect to see deer, we see deer, even if they are dressed in orange.

 

Stroop effectThe Stroop Effect is another good example of top-down processing. If I give you a list of colors all written in black, and ask you to read them aloud, you probably would have no difficulty doing so. If I give you a list of colors all written in their respective colors, you probably would have no difficult saying them aloud. If I give you list of colors written in the wrong colors and ask you to say the color they are written in (not the word but the color), you will find you’re quite slow at processing this information. Your brain recognizes them as words and wants to process them as words. This is top-down processing.

Similarly, if I show you an ambiguous item, you will use context to decide it if is a word or a number. Our brains actively look for words. We process words and numbers in different regions, so we look ahead to see what is coming our way. We decide if it is a b or a 13 depending on its context.

We use context to extract meaning when we read. When movie reviews or political ads take comments out of context, they misrepresent the statement because they have stripped it of its context.

In addition to all of the external cues, internal cues impact us too. In contrast to the external context-dependent cues (location, noise, music, wall colors, etc.), we also experience internal states, such as mood and pain. These internal factors are called state-dependent cues.

The Encoding Specificity Principle suggests that internal states can act as cues. In general, things that are emotional are more easily remembered. Advertisers and politicians often seek to make you feel something so you will remember their message better.

In 1964, Lyndon Johnson was in a race again Barry Goldwater for the American Presidency. Johnson’s campaign ran a television ad called Daisy. It opens with a 3-year old in a field with chirping birds. She is counting until a male voice begins a ballistic missile countdown and an atomic bomb is exploded. Johnson’s voice never mentions Goldwater but the impression is that Goldwater will bring on a nuclear war and kill all of your children.

Not quite as dramatically, advertisers show people having fun while using their products. The context and subtext is that if you use their products you too will be good looking, have plenty of dates, become rich and relax on a warm beach.

Emotion impacts our recall. When you are happy, you remember best the things you learned when you were happy. When you are depressed, you remember all of the ways people have been mean to you in the past, how lousy your life currently is, and how bleak your future looks.

An obvious application of state-dependent learning is how drugs impact us. When you are on your prescribed drugs, the things you learn will be remembered in that state. But if you go off your bipolar medications, you are unlikely to remember the same material well. When you go back on your meds, you remember the original material again.

When sober, people are not always able to remember where they put their keys when they were drunk. Getting drunk to remember won’t help because alcohol inhibits the encoding process. You probably didn’t learn much when you were drunk. The memory systems weren’t fully engaged.

Internal states include human motivation. Are you trying to meet basic needs or is your mental state focused on belonging and self-actualization? Your goals, wants and needs are all part of your internal mental context. They too impact your learning.

Filed Under: Learning

April 5, 2023 by ktangen

Encoding Specificity Principle

Encoding specificity principle in action: practice is realistic settings

The Encoding Specificity Principle has its roots in the earliest research of memory. When Ebbinghuas first used lists of words, he noted that associations between items aids recall. The internal context of the list matters. We look for any connection that helps us combine items into meaningful units.

Building on the work of Ebbinghaus, researchers using lists on nonsense syllables soon discovered that in addition to internal context, external context makes a difference. The environment can help you generate cues, and when you try to remember in a different environment (with different cues), it is more difficult.

The solution is to choose an encoding environment that is similar to where you must recall it. If you have to remember words on stage, it is best to learn them on stage. If you must remember how to do something under stress, practice it under stress. If you must take a test in a quiet environment, it is best to study in a quiet environment.

Tulving called this general principle of matching encoding and decoding contexts the Encoding Specificity Principle. It says that, in general, it is best to match your encoding context to the recall environment. If you must take a test without caffeine, it is best to study without caffeine. If you must perform in a hot environment, practice in a hot environment.

Tulving showed that retrieving episodic memories (stories of your life) is best when you recall them where you learned them. This is why people who return to their home town or old college can be flooded with memories. Cues they haven’t experienced for years are suddenly present, acting like magic keys to release locked memories. Being in the place where you learned things helps recall them.

For example, it is easiest to remember where your keys are when you are in the same room as they are. This is why when you’ve lost an idea, you go back to where you first had it and it reappears. The location context was giving you cues. Retracing your steps triggers those original cues that were present at encoding.

Location turn out to provide strong cues. This is why people in drug rehab find it so difficult to stay clean in their old haunts. Cues can act as triggers of emotional responses and, ultimately, a return to drug use. Stay out of places where you learned to be a druggie.

British memory researcher Alan Baddeley investigated cue-dependency by studying deep sea divers. The theory was that cold (the deeper you go underwater) acts a cue for learning. Eighteen divers did each of four conditions, counterbalancing the order is which they were presented. Since the divers each acted as their own control, a larger sample wasn’t needed.

The testing conditions were:

  • learning under water (20 feet below the surface) and recalling underwater
  • learning under water, recalling on land (by the shore)
  • learning on land, recalling on land
  • learning on land, recalling underwater

In this free recall test, divers remembered best when the learning and remembering conditions matched. The location context cues mattered. It didn’t matter whether it was underwater or on shore, only that the encoding and decoding contexts were the same.

The encoding specificity principle applies to music, smells, and ambient lighting. Matching input environment to output environment improves recall. When in doubt, match them.

But these external context-dependent cues aren’t immutable. You can overcome them. There are two main approaches to reinstating environmental context cues.

First, you can imagine you are underwater when you’re on shore. Baddeley had his divers close their eyes and visualize being underwater. Visualizing removed most of the blockage. Performance wasn’t as good as being underwater but quite close.

Obviously, your ability to visualize makes a difference. If can easily picture yourself in a location, mood or situation, you will do better than those who are less visually oriented. The trick is to consciously generate the environmental cues you’ll need at recall. It may help to take a picture of your classroom from where you sit and prop it up on your desk while you study.

Second, use multiple contexts. Study under quiet and noisy conditions. Study under bright and dim lighting. Study for recognition and recall. The more variations of environmental cues, the less impact they have. If you study in many contexts, none will overpower you.

The Encoding Specificity Principle extends to learning skills too. Learning transfers best between skills that are similar. Learning to sing probably won’t help your tap dancing. Learning your multiplication tables probably won’t help you carve pumpkins. The two skills are too far apart. Far transfer doesn’t work.

In contrast to the far transfer of widely different skills, near transfer aids performance. Waving your arms directing traffic will aid the arm waving ability to conduct an orchestra. The same muscles will get exercised. The same fluid motions will be trained. Remember Pat Morita’s role as Mr. Miyagi? Wax on and wax off are similar arm actions to the defensive moves in karate. This is near transfer.

The caution with near transfer is that it is best to avoid major incompatibilities. In the early days of aviation history, Beechcraft and Cessna had different control systems. In one you pushed the yoke forward to go up. In the other, you pulled it back. Pilots trained in one plane did fine flying the other plane until there was an emergency. When panic set in, pilots would push the controls in the wrong direction. My pilot friends tell me I made up this story but I like it anyway.

In general, pilots should fly. Dancers should practice more on their performance stages. Church choirs should practice where they will perform, not in a rehearsal hall in another building. Actors should have more dress rehearsals. Soldiers should have war games. Identify the performance conditions and match your training to them.

 

Photo by Specna Arms on Unsplash

Filed Under: Learning

April 5, 2023 by ktangen

Jefferson Out O f Context


Sometimes excepts of longer passages are good summaries. They give you the essence of the speech or book without requiring you to consume the whole tome.

But excepts can be misleading. A larger context provides the semantic cues and linguistic interactions to better interpret the meaning. In this sense, context gives meaning. [Read more…] about Jefferson Out O f Context

Filed Under: Learning

April 5, 2023 by ktangen

Learning Test 3 Terms

Test 3 Key Terms

  • ABC theory of personality
  • active listening
  • Adler
  • age of onset
  • Allport
  • amplification
  • anal stage
  • analytical psychology
  • anchoring
  • anger
  • anterograde amnesia
  • anxiety
  • anxiety hierarchies
  • archetypes
  • Beck
  • behaviorism
  • Big Five
  • bipolar
  • birth order
  • cardinal traits
  • castration anxiety
  • client-centered therapy
  • cognitive dissonance
  • collective unconscious
  • common sense theory of emotion
  • common traits
  • conditional positive regard
  • conformity
  • conscience
  • cryptomnesia
  • déjà vu
  • delusions
  • denial
  • depressants
  • depression
  • diffusion of responsibility
  • disgust
  • display rules
  • dissimilative stimuli
  • distorted cognitive rules
  • dopamine
  • ego
  • ego ideal
  • Ellis
  • existentialism
  • fear
  • Festinger
  • free association
  • functional fixedness
  • generalized anxiety
  • genital stage
  • glutamate
  • hallucinations
  • happiness
  • hierarchy of needs
  • himsteria
  • humanism
  • hypnotism
  • hysteria
  • id
  • in vivo desensitization
  • James-Lange theory of emotion
  • latency stage
  • Maslow
  • Milgram’s “electric shock” experiment
  • multiple personalities
  • narcissism
  • negative symptoms of schizophrenia
  • neuroticism
  • Oedipal complex
  • oral stage
  • overlapping clusters
  • penis envy
  • personal unconscious
  • phallic stage
  • phobia
  • positive psychology
  • positive symptoms of schizophrenia
  • posttraumatic growth
  • primordial fears
  • proprium
  • psychodynamic
  • psychological fugues
  • PTSD
  • rationalization
  • reciprocal determinism
  • Robber’s Cave experiment
  • Rogers
  • schedules of reinforcement
  • schemas
  • schizophrenia
  • self concept
  • self-actualization
  • Seligman’s PERMA
  • social loafing
  • splitting
  • stimulants
  • superego
  • surprise
  • systematic desensitization
  • thought insertion
  • trait theory
  • transference
  • trephining
  • unconditional positive regard
  • unconscious urges
  • volition
  • Wolpe
  • Wundt
  • Zimbardo’s Stanford prison experiment

Filed Under: Learning

April 5, 2023 by ktangen

Thorndike’ s Accomplishments

  • 1. Founded connectionism
    • Through experience, neural bonds or connections are formed between perceived stimuli and emitted responses
    • Intellect facilitates formation of the neural bonds
    • People of higher intellect can form more bonds
    • People of higher intellect form bonds easier
    • The ability to form bonds is rooted in genetic potential through the genes’ influence on the structure of the brain
    • The content of intellect is a function of experience and cultural background
  • 2. Conducted the first animal lab studies
    • Research on cats in puzzle boxes
    • Trial-and-error learning
    • Cats escaped by trying various behaviors until hit on the one solution that worked
    • Discard all non-solution behaviors
    • “Stamp in” correct connection
  • 3. Proposed multifactor theory of intelligence
    • During the 1920’s
    • CAVD Test of Intelligence
    • Completion
    • Arithmetic
    • Vocabulary
    • Directions
  • 4. Popularized adult education
  • 5. Changed “trained mind” to “transferable skills”
    • Locke’s Doctrine of Formal Discipline
    • John Locke (1632-1704)
      • Empiricism = ideas originate with sensory experience
      • No innate ideas
      • Blank slate
      • The mind is like a muscle
      • You have to exercise it to make it stronger
      • Transfer depends on the amount of effort you put into mastering a task
      • To reason well, a man must exercise his mind by observing the connection of ideas and following them in train
    • John & John Stuart Mill “Train of thought”
      • Nothing does this better than mathematics
      • It should be taught to all those who have the time and opportunity
      • Not to make them mathematicians
      • But to make them “reasonable creatures”
      • Once the mind is trained, they will be able to transfer their reasoning skills to other areas of knowledge
    • Thorndike’s Theory of Identical Elements
      • Transfer takes place when the original task is similar to the transfer task
      • More similarity, more transfer
      • It depends on how many “elements” the two tasks have in common
      • Taking a high school course in geometry
      • Won’t strengthen a general ability to think logically
      • May help you later in life
      • If you become a surveyor or navigator
      • Won’t help you if you become a lawyer
  • 6. Laid the groundwork for behaviorism
    • Objective experimental approach
  • 7. Laid groundwork for operant conditioning
    • Law of Effect
  • 8. Laid the groundwork for psychometrics
    • Used factor analysis before there were computers
  • 9. Founded educational psychology

Filed Under: History, Learning

April 5, 2023 by ktangen

What Are Schema

What Are Schemas?

Things in a box

When my daughter was young, she didn’t know “thingamabobs” was what people would say when they couldn’t think of the proper word for a collection of objects. Instead of calling it “that pile of stuff,” people would say “Clean up those thingamabobs.”

The term appears in Disney’s move The Little Mermaid.  Arial proudly states that if you want thingamabobs, she has 20. But who cares, no big deal, she wants more.

When my daughter sang the song, she changed the lyrics to fit her level of knowledge. She said that if you want things in a box, she has twenty. Things in a box made sense to her. Thingamabobs meant nothing.

Schemas are part of this adaptive quality we have of fitting the world into our personal viewpoint. From our experience, we create cognitive structures (for want of a better term). We put ideas into categories and use them to evaluate incoming information.

What Are Schemas?

Schemas are mental models that represent our best understanding of the world. Our personal experiences are different, so our schemas are different. If you know terms like thingamabobs and thingamajigs, your schema will include them. As your knowledge base grows, your schemas expand.

We start with a simple schema for animals. When we are little, all animals are referred to as dog or cat or duck. As we gain more experience, we add more information. We learn some cats are friendly and some are touchy. You add this information to your knowledge base. This is called assimilation. You add more information to a single category. It’s like putting more things in your house.

Eventually, your house is full and you need to buy an additional one (house sizes are limited in this metaphor). While that would be an expensive solution in real life, for cognitive structures we add new categories. This is called accommodation. After you’ve acquired a lot of information, you begin to differentiate between them. You accommodate by adding new categories. We add categories for live cats, fictional cats, cartoon cats, book cats, neighbor cats and so on.

We have a schema for a department store. Even if you make most of your purchases online and don’t go to the mall, you have a mental image of what a department store would include. For me, I think of how perfume counters seem to be located at the mall entrance, and how I have to hold my breath to get past them. I expect other customers to be present, sales representatives walking around, and music (live or piped) to be playing. I expect it to be air conditioned, large and to have tall ceilings. I expect both escalators and elevators. Your schema may differ.

How Do Schemas Work?

We use schema to organize our knowledge and to interpret new information. I visited a mall in France and found it to easily fit my schema for shopping malls. But my schema for grocery store and bakery had to be adjusted.

St. Peter's SquareIt is hard to change schemas that are based on only a few examples. My idea of a town square is the Grand Place in Brussels. It is the only example I have of large open space between old buildings. I’ve been to Time Square in New York but it doesn’t feel like a square to me. From pictures I’ve seen, Tiananmen Square in Beijing is too big. The Piazza del Campo in Siena, Tuscany, Italy is heart shaped. And St. Pete’s Square has round sides. If it has round sides, how can it really be a square?

I use the Grand Place to judge all other squares. That’s the advantage of schemas. We can use them to both organize our knowledge and to interpret new information. Once we have a good category, we use it and try not to change it.

Schemas develop over time. They are a product of our experience, interests and explorations. They build up. As we travel around our neighborhood, our town, our county and state, we gather more information to incorporate in our schemas. Since we travel different routes and go to different places, our schemas are unique to us.

Although unique to the individual, some schemas are common in a culture. We have categories for left-wing vs right-wing, for rich or poor, and for popular-unpopular. There are schemas for police officer, judge, professor, student and movie star. Each comes with certain expectations. Exceptions to those expectations make us add more detail to the schema (assimilation) or add a new category (accommodation).

When Are Schemas Used?

Schemas are activated by stimulus features. When people see a young person with white hair, pale skin and poor vision, their “albino” schema is activated. If they haven’t met an albino before, they may have to add a new category for albino-real. As an albino, it simply adds more information to my albino-like-me schema.

The fewer the samples, the stronger the schema. If you meet someone with red hair who is rude to you, your whole schema for red-haired people is based on one sample. If the next redhead you meet is also rude, the rude-redhead schema is even stronger. It takes many more samples of non-rude redheads to establish a new category. This is why it is difficult to change prejudices and stereotypes.

Once a feature is detected a schema is automatically triggered. If you hear the word “clown,” you automatically generate a reaction. It might be vague or specific but there is nothing you need to do for it to surface. Any sensory input can trigger a schema: vision, sound, touch and smell. All the senses can be involved, individually or collectively.

Some schemas are context sensitive and others are chronically accessible. The chronically and readily available schemas include our view of ourselves, our basic prejudices and our world view. Context sensitive schema include priming (hints given ahead of time), people and locations. Priming is hearing a word (water) and then hearing bank. You are more likely to remember river bank than you are bank teller.

People and location factors often impact us when we go home or are around our relatives. We are strong, independent people, except when we get around our siblings or elderly aunts. People and places can trigger old schemas.

Types of Schema

There are three types of schema: person, self and script. A person schema consists of your general knowledge and beliefs about other people traits and characteristics. Are people generally helpful, friendly and honest? Are people selfish, mean and dishonest? People schema are generalizations about people. We use them to organize our thinking and plan our behaviors (smile or hide).

Self-schemas are person schemas about you. They consist of your general knowledge about who you are. They are a collection of beliefs you have about your own personality, abilities and goals.

Scripts are event schemas. They contain your knowledge of interpersonal behaviors and what to do where. You have schemas for restaurants. You know not to wait for a hostess at a burger joint, and not to seat yourself at a fancy bistro. You know when to pay (before or after you get the food), what the server will say (“Would you like to hear our specials?”), whether they will bring you water, if you have to ask for fries, and whether it is proper to talk to people at the table next to you.

Scripts are like movie scripts: they allow you to anticipate events and fill in missing details. Take our typical conversational greeting script. First, someone says hello to you. Then you say hello back. Then they say “How are you?” And you will say…

They aren’t actually asking how you are. They don’t want to know about your aches, pains, upset stomach and rashes. They say it because it is part of the script.

Next time someone asks “How are you?” say “Good to see you.” You will discover how closely people follow scripts. The conversation will probably falter because you didn’t follow the script.

Is It Schema or Schemata?

You can use schemata for an individual schema, and schema for the plural. Or you can use schema for singular and schemas for plural. I use whichever the spellchecker won’t complain about. I have a pretty flexible schema for schemas.

Filed Under: Learning

  • « Go to Previous Page
  • Page 1
  • Page 2
  • Page 3
  • Page 4
  • Page 5
  • Page 6
  • Interim pages omitted …
  • Page 11
  • Go to Next Page »

Footer

Search

KenTangen.com

My Channel

Copyright © 2025 · Executive Pro on Genesis Framework · WordPress · Log in